Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sexual dichromatism in the neotropical genus Mannophryne (Anura: Aromobatidae)

  • Mark S. Greener ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    mark.s.greener@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Pathology, Bacteriology and Avian Diseases, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Ghent University, Merelbeke, Belgium

  • Emily Hutton,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom

  • Christopher J. Pollock,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation School of Biology, Faculty of Biological sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom

  • Annabeth Wilson,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom

  • Chun Yin Lam,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom

  • Mohsen Nokhbatolfoghahai,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom

  • Michael J. Jowers,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation CIBIO/InBIO (Centro de Investigacao em Biodiversidade e Recursos Genticos), Universide do Porto, Vairao, Portugal

  • J. Roger Downie

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom

Correction

3 Sep 2020: Greener MS, Hutton E, Pollock CJ, Wilson A, Lam CY, et al. (2020) Correction: Sexual dichromatism in the neotropical genus Mannophryne (Anura: Aromobatidae). PLOS ONE 15(9): e0238962. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238962 View correction

Abstract

Recent reviews on sexual dichromatism in frogs included Mannophryne trinitatis as the only example they could find of dynamic dichromatism (males turn black when calling) within the family Aromobatidae and found no example of ontogenetic dichromatism in this group. We demonstrate ontogenetic dichromatism in M. trinitatis by rearing post-metamorphic froglets to near maturity: the throats of all individuals started as grey coloured; at around seven weeks, the throat became pale yellow in some, and more strongly yellow as development proceeded; the throats of adults are grey in males and variably bright yellow in females, backed by a dark collar. We demonstrated the degree of throat colour variability by analysing a large sample of females. The red: green (R:G) ratio ranged from ~1.1 to 1.4, reflecting variation from yellow to yellow/orange, and there was also variation in the tone and width of the dark collar, and in the extent to which the yellow colouration occurred posterior to the collar. Female M. trinitatis are known to be territorial in behaviour. We show a positive relationship between throat colour (R:G ratio) and escape performance, as a proxy for quality. Our field observations on Tobago’s M. olmonae showed variability in female throat colour and confirmed that males in this species also turn black when calling. Our literature review of the 20 Mannophryne species so far named showed that all females have yellow throats with dark collars, and that male colour change to black when calling has been reported in eight species; in the remaining 12 species, descriptions of males calling are usually lacking so far. We predict that both dynamic and ontogenetic sexual dichromatism are universal in this genus and provide discussion of the ecological role of dichromatism in this genus of predominantly diurnal, non-toxic frogs, with strong paternal care of offspring.

Introduction

Most anuran amphibians are active mainly at night and intra-specific communication is mainly mediated by acoustic signals [1]. However, as more complexity in anuran behaviour is found, a wide diversity of visual signalling (movements, colours, patterns, shapes) both in diurnal and in nocturnal species is becoming established. For example, Rojas [2] reviewed the roles of colours and patterns; Hödl and Amezquita [3] reviewed and classified the variety of visual signals, and Starnberger et al. [4] discussed the multimodal roles of the vocal sac in signalling: not only auditory, but also visual and chemical in some cases. One category of visual signals involves sexual dichromatism, reviewed by Bell and Zamudio [5]. They distinguished two types. First, dynamic dichromatism, restricted to males, where the male develops a temporary colour signal related to courtship and breeding. The review identified 31 species in nine families where this occurred. Second, ontogenetic dichromatism, where either males or females develop a permanent colour difference as they mature. The review found this reported from 92 species in 18 families. Bell et al. [6] extended the dataset for dynamic dichromatism to 178 species in 15 families and subfamilies. Bell and Zamudio [5] found no species reported as having both sexual and ontogenetic dichromatism. However, a recent paper by Engelbrecht‐Wiggans and Tumulty [7] has documented both in the aromobatid frog Anomaloglossus beebei.

Dynamic sexual dichromatism is likely to be an aspect of sexual selection, where the male’s temporary colour in some way attracts females. Most frogs attract mates through acoustic signals and breed at night, when a colour signal would appear not to be useful. However, in some diurnal species there is evidence that colour can be used by females to assess male quality in nocturnal breeding aggregations where discrimination of acoustic signals is difficult [8]. This may not always be the case, as in Rana arvalis that undergo a temporary colour change to blue during explosive breeding events for several days of the year, it was found that colouration was used to used for sex recognition by males [9].

Bell and Zamudio [5] included two classes of ontogenetic dichromatism. In the first, where males develop more conspicuous colouration than females, sexual selection is likely to be the main driver, with an expectation that the permanent colour should not be at significant cost. An example is the poison frog Oophaga pumilio where the male’s bright colour attracts females but also acts as an aposematic signal, deterring predators [10]. In the second class, females develop brighter colours than the males: this may be explicable through sexual selection, but an alternative may be sexual niche partitioning, where the two sexes occupy different niches, and colour contributes in some way to successful occupation.

In this paper, we report on the occurrence of both dynamic and ontogenetic sexual dichromatism in frogs of the neotropical genus Mannophryne. Frost [11] lists 38 species in the subfamily Aromobatinae of which 20 belong to the genus Mannophryne. Mannophryne are ground-living frogs inhabiting the environs of mountain streams in Venezuela and the Caribbean islands of Trinidad and Tobago (West Indies). They are cryptically coloured, with dorsal sides mostly mottled grey and brown, and they lack the poison gland protection found in dendrobatids, to which they are closely related [12,13]. Most accounts find Mannophryne to be day active frogs, but there are occasional reports of them remaining active after dusk [14,15,this paper].

We focus on sexual dichromatism in two species, M. trinitatis and M. olmonae, but also review literature reports from other Mannophryne species. Kenny [16] noticed that when male M. trinitatis are calling, their colour changes from the normal cryptic mottled brown/grey to jet black. Wells [17] reported that the colour change is very rapid, occurring over 1–10 minutes, both at the start of calling and at the end of an episode of calling when the colour reverts to normal. Bell and Zamudio [5] included this as their only example of dynamic sexual dichromatism in the family Dendrobatidae (we follow Frost [11] in placing Mannophryne in the family Aromobatidae). Wells [17] noticed another form of sexual dichromatism in M. trinitatis. Females aggressively defend territories and display their pulsating bright yellow throats when they do so. The yellow patch is posteriorly bounded by a narrow dark collar (a pigmented band of variable tone and width, extending across the ventral surface at the level of the forelimbs). Males possess the collar, but the throat is grey and Wells comments that it is not pulsated during aggressive encounters between males. Bright yellows are often based on carotenoid pigments, regarded as costly to synthesise, and in many taxa, including some anurans, they have been associated with signals of quality (reviewed by Olson & Owens [18]; tree frog example: Richardson et al. [19]).

In this paper, we assess the variability of the yellow pigmentation in a large sample of M. trinitatis females, and report on an experiment where we used escape responses as a proxy for quality. We also document the development of the yellow throat colour as metamorphs grow towards maturity. Finally, we provide field observations on the behaviour of both M. trinitatis and M. olmonae, and review what has been reported on sexual dichromatism in other Mannophryne species.

Materials and methods

Colour variability in female Mannophryne trinitatis

A large sample (n = 500) of adult female M. trinitatis was collected from different localities (Fig 1, S1 Table) in Trinidad’s Northern Range during June to August 2015 and 2016. Females, recognised by having yellow throats, were caught either by hand or with the aid of small hand-nets and placed for transport into small polyethylene bags containing a little damp forest leaf litter. They were kept overnight in a holding tank furnished with damp leaf litter, at the University of the West Indies, St. Augustine. The frogs were photographed and measured (snout-vent length [SVL] to 0.1 mm using dial callipers) on the day after collection, and then returned to their collection sites within ~20m of capture location. Despite an earlier report of the presence of the potentially deadly fungal pathogen Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd) in the M. trinitatis population, Greener et al. [20] found the infection to be absent. Capturing and returning the frogs should therefore not risk spreading infection.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Map of sites used in field studies.

M. trinitatis sampling sites, as detailed in S1 Table. Adapted from Greener et al. [20].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g001

To photograph the throat region, each frog was carefully held upside down and the ventral side photographed on a white background beside an X-Rite Colorchecker passport colour rendition chart (X-Rite Inc., Michigan, USA). All photographs were taken using a Sony ILCE-6000 with an attached Sony E-Mount SEL 55-200mm lens and lens hood (2015) or SEL 18-55mm lens (2016). The camera was set with F8.0 aperture, and shutter speed adjusted to give an exposure of 0.0. Photographs were taken in a white light box studio. Constant light was provided by three LED lights positioned on the right, left and above, along with an overhead strip light. All batches (frogs caught that day) of photographs were taken within one hour of each other. As in Stevens et al. [21], RAW format was chosen, as opposed to JPEG–as used by Bergman and Beehner [22]–as this prevents loss of information due to compression, allows for later adjustment, and due to current storage capabilities, file size was not an issue. The photograph from the first frog in each batch of frogs was used to create a colour profile using the software accompanying the X-Rite Colorchecker. A profile allows for colour correction given the same conditions. This profile was then applied to all photographs within the batch in Adobe Photoshop CC 2015, and white balance corrected for each photograph. The area of interest (yellow throat region) was selected, and average blurred before red (R) and green (G) values were recorded. These values were then used to generate R:G ratios. The presence of yellow colouration under the collar was also qualitatively assessed as Yes or No. We modelled the relationship between R:G ratio and SVL, site and presence of colouration under the collar using generalized linear mixed models, including year as a random effect, with statistical significance evaluated by Wald chi-squared testc. All analysis was conducted in RStudio 1.2.5033 environment R3.6.2 [23].

Colour development in juvenile Mannophryne trinitatis

A sample of about 60 M. trinitatis tadpoles was collected in early July 2016 by hand netting from a pool in a stream beside the Arima-Blanchisseuse Road in Trinidad’s Northern Range mountains. This was a sample of a larger number of tadpoles within the pool. The large number of tadpoles implies that many clutches had been deposited and therefore our sample had offspring from multiple parents. The tadpoles were transferred to the University of Glasgow, Scotland in two-litre polyethylene containers with the tadpoles resting on damp cotton cloth. Downie and Smith [24] showed that these tadpoles survive well under such conditions. In Glasgow, the tadpoles were grown at an initial density of 30 individuals per tank in plastic aquaria 32x18x18 cm in dechlorinated tap water at a depth of 10 cm and with a constant air supply delivered through a submerged air-stone at one end of the tank. The room where the aquaria were located had a 12:12h light/dark cycle and an air temperature of 23–24°C. The tadpoles were fed daily with aquarium fish food flakes (New Era brand), and the water was changed weekly to avoid the build-up of waste. Each day, the aquaria were checked for tadpoles showing forelimb emergence, the sign of metamorphosis beginning. The first metamorph was found on 8th July and the last on 16th September. Each metamorph was caught by hand-net and transferred to an individual translucent 22x15x8 cm polyethylene container with an opaque lid. Each container was provided with a ‘shelf’ of washed gravel at one end, about 18 cm wide and 1–2 cm deep, and dechlorinated tap water to a depth of about 0.5 cm. Downie et al. [25] have shown that M. trinitatis take 6–7 days to complete metamorphosis and that they hide in gravel during that time.

After completion of metamorphosis, each froglet was provided twice a week with live Drosophila melanogaster as food, supplied in 5 cm long tubes, which the frogs could enter to forage. Froglets were measured (SVL, using callipers accurate to 0.1 mm) and their throat patterns noted and/or photographed, starting about two weeks after forelimb emergence and continuing at approximately every four weeks for 16 weeks. For this purpose, froglets were captured by hand, transferred to a transparent 9 cm diameter petri dish, and measured from below. Once the 16-week growth period was complete, each frog was transferred to a communal tank for rearing to adulthood.

Escape responses in Mannophryne trinitatis in relation to female throat colour

Adult female (n = 81) M. trinitatis were captured with the aid of small hand-nets from five sites across Trinidad’s Northern Range (Fig 1, S1 Table), during mornings over six weeks, June to August 2018. Sites were visited in rotation, with 5–6 frogs captured at each visit. Frogs were transported to our laboratory at the William Beebe Tropical Research Station (Simla) and housed individually in plastic aquaria furnished with a thick layer of damp forest leaf litter. Frogs were measured (SVL to 0.1 mm with dial callipers; weight to 0.01g using an electronic balance) and any with SVL <16mm were classed as juveniles and excluded from further study. The day after collection, frogs were photographed in the morning around 09.00h and repeated at night around 21.00h: three pictures of the throat were taken using a Canon PowerShot s110 digital camera, in a white light box studio with two LED lamps providing constant light from either side of the light box. The camera was set with F3.2 aperture, and shutter speed adjusted to give an exposure of 0.0, allowing for comparison with throat colour variation measured earlier. Dorsal sides of each frog were also photographed to identify frogs, so as to ensure that no frog was used more than once, following subsequent collections (dorsal patterns are individually variable). Throat colour, as R:G ratio, was measured as described earlier.

Escape responses were assessed two mornings after collection (09.00–12.00h) in a specially constructed outdoor arena set in a shaded area: this had wooden sides about 0.8m high (to prevent frogs escaping) and enclosed an area of short grass 1.5x1.5m. Each frog was put into a 9cm diameter plastic petri dish and placed at the centre of the arena; it was left there for 30 seconds with the lid off to acclimatise. If the frog jumped before 30 seconds were over, it was recaptured and left a further 30 seconds. The frog was then stimulated by a light tap to the rear using a metre stick. Each frog’s responses, three times for each frog, were recorded for 20 seconds using a GoPro HERO6 video camera set above the arena. After each response, each frog was given at least 30 minutes to recover before being stimulated to jump again. Air temperature (0C) and relative humidity (%) within the arena were recorded using an ETI pocket thermo-hygrometer at the same times as each set of responses.

Image J (v1.52a) was used to measure the distance of each jump. From the recordings, we calculated maximum and minimum distances of each jump made, mean distance per jump and the total distance travelled in each trial (i.e. different measures of escape performance). Using RStudio 1.2.5033 environment R3.6.2 [23], general linear models were used to test relationships between escape performance and size (SVL), site, humidity, temperature and throat colour (as R:G ratio). The Benjamani–Hochberg procedure was used to correct for multiple testing. We expected escape performance (for example total distance jumped, or initial jump length) to be positively related both to size and throat colour, but to be independent of collection site, humidity and temperature.

New field observations on Mannophryne trinitatis

While assessing the population status of M. trinitatis for Greener et al. [20], we made occasional interesting observations relevant to colour and behaviour, both during the day and at dusk/night, which we present here.

Sexual dichromatism in Mannophryne olmonae

Field observations on M. olmonae were made at several small un-named streams in northeast Tobago in June to August 2014 and 2015. In 2015, female M. olmonae were captured using hand-nets and transferred in individual containers to accommodation in Charlotteville. Here, they were photographed alongside an X-rite Colour Checker rendition chart, under identical lighting conditions, using a Canon EOS Rebel T3i. Throat colour, as R:G ratio, was measured as described earlier and frogs were returned to their collection sites. As in the case of M. trinitatis, Thomson et al. [26] have shown Bd to be absent from the M. olmonae population.

Comparison of sexual dichromatism across the genus Mannophryne

We performed a literature review on male and female colours in life in all Mannophryne species so far described. The black colour in calling males can only be seen when observing males calling in the field; the female yellow throat colour fades in preservative. In some cases, colours in life are not presented in the original species descriptions, but we were often able to find later accounts of colours in life.

Ethics statement

A field research permit (Special Game License) was provided by Government’s Wildlife Section for the years of this study (2015, 2016 and 2018) for the months of June–August, no numbers are allocated. This permit allowed for the capture and use of animals in these studies for frogs of all species up to 500 adult individuals per year. Export of tadpoles was granted by Special Export License 001192 (29/6/16). The Special Export License we received from the Trinidad Wildlife Division allowed for capture of animals and the collection of 100 animals, with no distinguishing between adults or tadpoles, it also did not require them to be returned to the wild. No ethical approval was required as studies were non-invasive and very lows stress levels and considered under the threshold of the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act. All adult frogs (apart from a small number that died in captivity) were returned within a couple of days to their original location. Captured tadpoles were exported live to the UK, where they were reared to metamorphosis until adult. After this, they were donated to an experienced tropical frog breeder. No aspects of our study required anaesthesia, euthanasia or any kind of animal sacrifice.

Results

Throat colour variability in female Mannophryne trinitatis

Larger females were found to be more likely to have a higher R:G ratio on their throat patch (p <0.001) (Fig 2). Two of our sites, Edith Falls and Las Cuevas, were found to have a significant positive effect on R:G ratio (p <0.001). Females with high R:G ratios were found to be more likely to have colour posterior to the collar (p 0.037). However, SVL and its interaction with R:G ratio were not significant predictors of this. We qualitatively observed that the colouration of the throat area differed between individuals not only in R:G ratio but also in the relative size of the throat patch, the intensity and width of the dark collar, and the extent to which the yellow colour extended posterior to the collar (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 2. The relationship between throat patch R:G ratio and SVL (mm) in M. trinitatis for all sites.

Line indicates linear regression of all sites combined. The shaded area indicates the 95% confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Variation in the throat colouration of female M. trinitatis.

Variability in throat patch relative size and shape, collar width and intensity, and extension of the colour posterior to the collar. R:G denotes the Red:Green ratio as extracted from photographs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g003

Colour development in juvenile Mannophryne trinitatis

Forty-four of the tadpoles reached metamorphosis. Of these, 25 developed long enough for their sex to be distinguished by throat colour differences, 20 as females and five as males. This sex ratio is significantly biased towards females (chi squared = 15.8; p< 0.001). Table 1 shows size and colour development data for all froglets recorded beyond 90 days post metamorphosis (omitting two females that escaped at around 80 days). Dark collars developed in both males and females. Although these were variable in width and shade, we saw no consistent difference between males and females. Throat colour started and remained grey in five individuals throughout the observation period and these were classified as males (we did not check sex by examining gonads). In individuals developing as females, throat colour started as grey, became pale yellow at around seven weeks post metamorphosis, and either remained pale or became more brightly yellow around nine weeks (Fig 4). Since we did not assess throat colour weekly (too frequent disturbance could be stressful, and risked escapes), we cannot tell precisely when the yellow colour first became apparent. However, throats were pale grey in all individuals at the first set of observations (2–3 weeks after metamorphosis began) and remained grey at 5–6 weeks in individuals that developed as females. These results indicate that throat colour is ontogenetically sexually dichromatic, developing in the juvenile phase, well before female maturation (mature females are around 20mm SVL; the yellow throat was distinguishable at around 15.5 mm).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Throat colour patch development in a selection of post-metamorphic M. trinitatis reared in captivity.

d.p.m = days post metamorphosis. Early development left hand column; later stages of the same froglets to the right. Early stages show slight or no yellow pigmentation. Later stages all with yellow throats, female, except frog 5 J, male.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g004

thumbnail
Table 1. The appearance of coloured throats in post-metamorphic M. trinitatis reared in captivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.t001

Escape responses in Mannophryne trinitatis in relation to female throat colour

Responses to stimulation were quite varied. Some frogs made only a few jumps before stopping on the grass; others jumped to the edge of the arena and climbed some way up the wall (Mannophryne have adhesive toe pads); the direction of jumping was also variable but most tended to maintain more or less the same direction once they set off.

Comparison of morning and night photographs of frog throats indicated that the colour was stable, with no diurnal variation. Table 2 shows female frog sizes, colour variability and escape performance. There were no significant differences between collection sites and escape performance (p>0.05 for all measures). Air temperature during the trials had a range of only 1.8°C (26.9–28.7°C), but humidity varied more widely (53–86%). There were no significant relationships between temperature or humidity and escape performance (all p>0.05). Also, there was no significant relationship between collection site and throat colour (R:G ratio: p>0.05). However, there were strong and significant positive relationships between R:G ratio and frog size (weight: F = 18.42, p<0.001; SVL: F = 19.07, p< 0.001). Weight and SVL were also strongly correlated with one another (not shown).

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean(+/-SD) values for frog size, R:G ratio and measures of escape response.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.t002

We found a positive significant relationship between R:G ratio (throat colour) and the maximum distance travelled in a single jump (F = 11.55, p<0.01). One observation of throat colour (R:G = 1.61) had a noticeably larger R:G ratio than the rest (Fig 5), though this difference was not alarming as the relationship between maximum distance travelled and throat patch colour was still significant whether this datapoint was included or not for statistical analysis (p<0.01 in both cases). There were no significant relationships between R:G ratio and the other measures of escape performance: minimum distance in a single jump; mean distance per jump; initial jump distance; total distance travelled (p>0.05 in all cases).

thumbnail
Fig 5. The relationship between the maximum distance travelled in a single jump (mm) and throat patch colouration (R:G ratio) for M. trinitatis females.

The line shows linear regression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g005

Interestingly, we found no significant relationships between SVL and any of the escape performance measures (p>0.05 in all cases). S2 Table summarises the statistical results on escape performance.

New field observations on Mannophryne trinitatis

We observed M. trinitatis sites at dusk and during the hour after sunset. Some male Mannophryne remained active, calling, feeding and transporting tadpoles. Although numbers were fewer and calling less frequent than during the day, frogs were out in the open and not hard to find. During a day-time survey, we noticed a male in normal non-calling colouration calling occasionally i.e. not at the usual high frequency. As we watched, this frog began to change to black and over the same time, its calling frequency increased, the full change taking about 20 minutes.

Sexual dichromatism in Mannophryne olmonae

Our field observations confirm that M. olmonae males are black when calling. On a late afternoon in June 2014 we first observed a group of three males on a rock by a stream near the Charlotteville- Bloody Bay road (Tobago). All three were calling and all were black. One soon hopped away. The other two called facing one another until one other hopped away, leaving the ‘victor’ of the encounter (Fig 6A and 6B). We made many similar observations over the next four weeks, but did not actually observe the colour transformation from brownish to black, although we did see a calling male that was mainly brown dorsally and possibly at the start or end of the transition (Fig 6C and 6D). Of 47 calling males observed, 68% were on rocks, 17% on leaves and only 15% in crevices. In a further visit in 2015, we captured 12 adult females and photographed their throats for colour analysis. All had yellow throat patches with a narrow brownish collar and an R:G ratio ranging from 1.03 to 1.104.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Males of M. olmonae.

(A) two males, jet black all over, soon after a third male had hopped away. (B) the remaining male after the second one in (A) had hopped away; black colour already diminished. (C,D) males with inflated throats, but not yet black all over.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.g006

Comparison of sexual dichromatism across the genus Mannophryne

Table 3 shows the results of our literature search for evidence of both dynamic sexual dichromatism in males and sex differences in throat colour, assumed to be ontogenetic. We found that in 8 (maybe 9) out of 20 species, dynamic colour change has been documented. In a few cases, colour change in males has not been observed, but this is generally in species where calling has not been seen. We also found that in all cases, females displayed at least some yellow colouration in the throat region. The table includes any information found on the speed of colour change in males, but this has only been reported in a few species so far.

thumbnail
Table 3. Reported occurrences of ontogenetic and dynamic dichromatism in living specimens of Mannophryne species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.t003

All data used within paper can be found in S3S5 Tables.

Discussion

In this paper, focused on the Trinidad stream frog Mannophryne trinitatis, with additional observations on the Tobago stream frog M. olmonae, and a review of the colour descriptions of the other Mannophryne species so far identified, we show that a) dynamic sexual dichromatism is widespread in the genus, and b) that ontogenetic sexual dichromatism, principally involving the development of a bright yellow throat patch occurs throughout the genus in females. This highlights another example of frogs exhibiting both dichromatisms, this time across a whole genus. We show that the yellow throat patch is highly variable in M. trinitatis and M. olmonae, and that colour development begins soon after metamorphosis in M. trinitatis. We also provide a test that supports our hypothesis that throat colour provides a signal of female quality.

Reviews of the occurrence of conspicuous colouration in frogs [2,3] emphasise two general cases. First, aposematic (warning) signals to other species indicate that these frogs are well protected by toxins. A complication may arise where harmless species evolve to mimic the toxic species, gaining protection without incurring the costs of producing toxins. Second, to protect the frog from drawing the unwelcome attention of predators, the conspicuously coloured element is either temporary or concealed, except from the intended receiver.

Dynamic sexual dichromatism in male Mannophryne

Bell et al. [6] found a relationship between dynamic sexual dichromatism and explosive breeding aggregations in hylids, bufonids and some other groups, and suggested that colour change may assist mate recognition in such situations. However, they also noted that dynamic dichromatism occurs in the absence of breeding aggregations in some species, and related it to intraspecific competition, such as territory defence, in these cases. Colour change in frogs is generally found to be slow (hours to days) and mediated by hormones. However, Kindermann et al. [39] found that a dorsal change from brown to yellow in amplexing male Litoria wilcoxii took around 5 minutes, and that it could be induced in non-amplexing males by epinephrine injection, implying a neuroendocrine mechanism. Wells [17] reported that male M. trinitatis begin calling and then change to black within 1–10 minutes; we also noticed that males start to call before changing to black and that the change occurred over minutes. In other Mannophryne species, La Marca [28] reported M. cordilleriana changing to black a few seconds after starting to call, and Rojas-Runjaic et al. [15] made a similar observation on M. molinai. In other species where a change has been seen (8 out of 20 species, plus probably M. riveroi: Table 3), the rate of change has not been noted. In a few accounts, the return from black to brown has been reported to be similarly fast. Although no research has been reported on the mechanism of colour change in male Mannophryne, the speed implies a neuroendocrine process, and a study to test this is needed.

The function of the male change to black is unclear. Mannophryne trinitatis is not an explosive breeder: frogs are distributed along stream sides, with females holding long-term territories. Breeding can occur throughout the long wet season, presumably dependent on females having ripe egg clutches [16,17]. Mannophryne, although closely related to dendrobatids, lack toxic protection and are generally cryptic in colouration and behaviour. Their usual habitat is the margins of rocky streams in tropical forest, and they are mainly active during the day. Light levels are low, and the habitat provides abundant shaded crevices where frogs with mottled dorsal colouration of browns, greys and blacks are well concealed. Wells [17] found that males with normal brownish colouration never attacked other males, nor were they attacked by calling males. However, aggressive encounters between black calling males were common. Calling males did not appear to be particularly territorial in their behaviour, often changing calling site, usually a conspicuous position such as a rock or log (however, we have often seen males calling from shaded crevices). Since both calling and black colouration make the males conspicuous, it is unclear why both signals are needed, especially when they likely increase the risk of predation. Mimicry is unlikely to be at work here. Although some toxic frogs are conspicuously black (for example, dendrobatids of the genus Ameerega), their ranges do not appear to overlap with those of Mannophryne [40]. The key to the dynamic black signal in males may lie in the unusual territorial behaviour of female Mannophryne (see later). In order to attract a female, the males may need to demonstrate their own quality by having visibly successful encounters with other males, or by being conspicuous (colour and sound) for an extended period.

Bell et al. [6] checked 13 species of Mannophryne for the occurrence of colour change in the males and found that only M.trinitatis fitted their criteria, which required photographic evidence. However, we read original species descriptions and some later reports. Since the occurrence of the black colour is transient and not found in preserved specimens, definitive sightings require field observations of calling males. These are often lacking in reports and colour change is often only briefly referred to, since it cannot be used as a species identification criterion in preserved specimens. Of the 12 species reviewed by Bell et al. [6] as lacking colour change, we found colour change descriptions in three cases: Mannophryne herminae, M. olmonae and M. venezuelensis. In addition, the seven species not covered by Bell et al. yielded three cases of colour change: M. cordilleriana, M. larandina and M. molinai.

If male colour change occurs in some Mannophryne but not others, a possible explanation is phylogeny. Manzanilla et al. [41] analysed mitochondrial DNA sequences from 13 of the 15 Mannophryne then known. They identified three clades of five, one and three species respectively. More recently, Grant et al. [13] analysed 14 species and essentially confirmed Manzanilla et al.’s clades, though they noted some anomalies in the species so far identified. Male colour change while calling has been reported in species belonging to both of the larger clades. Given that all Mannophryne species appear to live in similar habitats with similar behavioural ecology, our hypothesis is that colour change in calling males is likely to occur throughout the genus, and that those species where it has not been reported have not yet been adequately observed in the field.

Ontogenetic sexual dichromatism in Mannophryne

Amongst the anurans, hyperoliids (reed frogs) are the group previously shown to commonly undergo ontogenetic sexual dichromatism, with females developing a bright colour while the males remain dull (found in 35 of 215 species [5,42]). As we have shown, ontogenetic dichromatism in female throat colour appears to occur throughout Mannophryne. Females have yellow throat patches of varying size and shade, backed by a dark collar of variable width and tone; the yellow patches sometimes extend beyond the collar on to the abdomen. Males may have a collar, but their throats are invariably grey to black, never yellow. Some species accounts refer to the colour patterns of juveniles, but our study presents the first account of ontogenetic changes in throat colour from the end of metamorphosis to near maturity, and it is clear that the adult sexual dichromatism is ontogenetic. Wells [17] found that female M. trinitatis aggressively defend their territories and that the signal used to denote a territory holder is pulsation of the throat with the head held high so that the yellow throat patch, pulsation and dark collar are clearly visible to any approaching conspecific. Durant and Dole [43] described similar behaviour in female Colostethus (now Mannophryne) collaris. Exposure of a coloured signal that is normally concealed has been infrequently reported in frogs. One other example is the brightly coloured foot webbing in the foot-flagging frog Staurois parvus, where males extend and rotate their legs, displaying the colours, during social interactions [44].

To our knowledge, no one has previously suggested that variation in the yellow colour acts as a signal of female quality. However, across the animal kingdom, bright yellow patches are often used in this way, related to the cost of synthesising the carotenoids on which yellow colours are often based, and to the role of carotenoids in immune system function [18]. In anurans, a relevant example is the orange-coloured (carotenoid-based) vocal sac of chorusing tree frogs, where females prefer males with colourful compared to pale vocal sacs [19,45].

Our data show considerable individual differences in female throat colour in both M. trinitatis and M. olmonae. To test whether throat colour differences act as signals of female quality, it would be best to recover the winners and losers after territorial encounters and measure their throat colours. However, in practice, the combination of the need to observe from a distance (to prevent disturbance) and the nature of the terrain (rocks with abundant deep crevices used by frogs for concealment) made this unfeasible. We used a proxy for quality instead. Royan et al. [46] reported experiments where the escape response trajectories of captured M. trinitatis were measured using an outdoor arena. They found that angle of escape was variable, indicating a degree of unpredictability, which could help individuals to escape potential predators. We reasoned that differences in escape response might provide as good a measure of quality as the results of territorial encounters. Our results showed a positive relationship between R:G ratio and two measures of escape performance, providing evidence that throat patch colour is indeed a signal of quality. However, the signal emitted by females is more than simply the colour of the throat. Our measurements over a large population of female M. trinitatis and a small sample of M. olmonae show variation in throat colour, patch size, the width and colour of the collar, and the extent of the yellow patch posterior to the collar so the quality of the signal may include all these components.

Conclusion and a hypothesis

The adaptive significance of female sexual dichromatism in Mannophryne seems clear: it is associated with territorial defence. Territorial behaviour in dendrobatids is common, but mainly involves males [2]. Long-term defence of a territory by female frogs, as occurs in Mannophryne (extrapolating from the species where it has been demonstrated, such as M. trinitatis and M. collaris), is very rare. The obvious suggestion is that females are defending resources, most likely food. Frogs captured in late afternoon showed that females had significantly fuller stomachs (small insects, arachnids and occasional snails) than males [47]. It is not known whether food resources are patchily distributed or simply related to area: a study on territory size in relation to throat colour would be helpful. If we are correct in concluding that throat colour differences in females provide a measure of quality, then it is likely that the throat pulsing observed by Wells [17] during female to female intraspecific confrontation provides a signal, potentially avoiding a physical confrontation. Additionally, it would be beneficial to the males to choose the best possible quality of mate, demonstrated by their visually striking territorial defence. Females also need to select the best possible mate: males guard the eggs throughout incubation and then transport the hatchlings to a suitable body of water. Downie et al. [48] found that transporting males may take several days to locate a suitable pool, ideally one lacking predators. This post-hatching transportation phase probably makes it unfeasible for male Mannophryne to guard multiple egg clutches, as occurs in some other clutch-guarders, such as glass frogs [49]. Males therefore have to be sure that the quality of the eggs is high enough to justify the considerable investment in time involved in incubation and transportation. Males can demonstrate their quality by calling for long periods, and by turning conspicuously black, both hazardous and possibly energetically expensive activities. The mating dances the males perform also contribute [17]. Our hypothesis therefore is that the occurrence of sexual dichromatism in both sexes derives from the resource-based territoriality of the females, using throat colouration as a signal in female to female conspecific confrontations, and strong selection for quality in both sexes. Males avoid the predation costs of conspicuousness by their colour signal being temporary and quickly turned off and on; for females, the colour signal is concealed, except when used against conspecific receivers. Follow up studies on the reproductive success, or even just territory defensive success, of females with different throat colouration would be very interesting. However, we are aware of how difficult this would be. Further studies on the development of the female colouration across multiple species and over a longer time period may lead to some interesting results. Clarification of the presence or absence of the dynamic colour change for males of species from the rest of the genus would also be useful.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Frog collection data.

Data for the number of individuals sampled from each site per year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Significance values from analysis of escape response trials.

Breakdown of the significance values from each statistical test conducted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Data from colour variability in female Mannophryne trinitatis section.

All data collected and used for the colour variability in female Mannophyne trinitatis study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.s003

(XLS)

S4 Table. Data from colour development in juvenile Mannophryne trinitatis.

All data collected and used for analysis in colour development in juvenile Mannophryne trinitatis study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Data from escape responses in Mannophryne trinitatis in relation to female throat colour.

All data collected and used for analysis in escape responses in Mannophryne trinitatis in relation to female throat colour study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223080.s005

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The fieldwork upon which the results reported here are based was carried out on University of Glasgow expeditions to Trinidad, and to Tobago over the years 2015–18, with many students helping make observations and catch frogs. Fieldwork permits were provided in Trinidad by the Government’s Wildlife Section, Forestry Division, in St. Joseph. In addition, Special Export License 001192 (29/6/16) gave permission for export of the tadpoles used in the rearing experiment. Thanks to Anna Bandoo and Romano McFarlane for facilitating these permits. In Tobago, Angela Ramsay of the Tobago House of Assembly’s Department of Natural Resources and the Environment provided fieldwork permits. Multiple agencies supported the expeditions financially, notably the University of Glasgow’s Chancellor’s Fund, Glasgow Natural History Society, the Gilchrist Educational Trust, the Thriplow Trust, and Dennis Curry’s Charitable Trust. A Percy Sladen Memorial Fund grant funded the camera used in the escape response experiments. The University of the West Indies and the Asa Wright Nature Centre provided laboratory accommodation in Trinidad, and Pat Turpin in Tobago. We thank Dr James D. Burgon for assistance on the technical and conceptual aspects. We thank Rayna C. Bell for her comments on an earlier version of this paper.

References

  1. 1. Wells KD. The Ecology and Behavior of Amphibians. 2007;
  2. 2. Rojas B. Behavioural, ecological, and evolutionary aspects of diversity in frog colour patterns. Biol Rev. 2017;92: 1059–1080. pmid:27020467
  3. 3. Hödl W, Amézquita A. Visual signaling in anuran amphibians. Anuran communication. 2001; 121–141.
  4. 4. Starnberger I, Preininger D, Hödl W. The anuran vocal sac: a tool for multimodal signalling. Anim Behav. 2014;97: 281–288. pmid:25389375
  5. 5. Bell RC, Zamudio KR. Sexual dichromatism in frogs: natural selection, sexual selection and unexpected diversity. Proc Biological Sci Royal Soc. 2012;279: 4687–93. pmid:22993253
  6. 6. Bell R, Webster G, Whiting M. Breeding biology and the evolution of dynamic sexual dichromatism in frogs. J Evolution Biol. 2017;30: 2104–2115. pmid:28833835
  7. 7. Engelbrecht‐Wiggans E, Tumulty JP. “Reverse” sexual dichromatism in a Neotropical frog. Ethology. 2019;125: 957–964.
  8. 8. Vásquez T, Pfennig KS. Looking on the bright side: females prefer coloration indicative of male size and condition in the sexually dichromatic spadefoot toad, Scaphiopus couchii. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2007;62: 127–135.
  9. 9. Sztatecsny M, Preininger D, Freudmann A, Loretto M-C, Maier F, Hödl W. Don’t get the blues: conspicuous nuptial colouration of male moor frogs (Rana arvalis) supports visual mate recognition during scramble competition in large breeding aggregations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012;66: 1587–1593. pmid:23162205
  10. 10. Maan ME, Cummings ME. Sexual dimorphism and directional sexual selection on aposematic signals in a poison frog. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106: 19072–19077.
  11. 11. Frost DR. Amphibian Species of the World: an Online Reference. Version 6.0. 2020;
  12. 12. GRANT T, FROST DR, CALDWELL JP, GAGLIARDO R, HADDAD C, KOK I, et al. PHYLOGENETIC SYSTEMATICS OF DART-POISON FROGS AND THEIR RELATIVES (AMPHIBIA: ATHESPHATANURA: DENDROBATIDAE). B Am Mus Nat Hist. 2006;299: 1–262.
  13. 13. Grant T, Rada M, Anganoy-Criollo M, Batista A, Dias P, Jeckel A, et al. Phylogenetic Systematics of Dart-Poison Frogs and Their Relatives Revisited (Anura: Dendrobatoidea). S Am J Herpetol. 2017;12: S1–S90.
  14. 14. Barrio-Amorós C, Santos J, Molina CR. Adição à diversidade de dendrobatídeos da Venezuela: descrição de três novas espécies de Mannophryne (Anura: Dendrobatidae). Phyllomedusa J Herpetology. 2010;9: 03–35.
  15. 15. Rojas-Runjaic FJ, Matta-Pereira ME, la Marca E. Unveiling species diversity in collared frogs through morphological and bioacoustic evidence: a new Mannophryne (Amphibia, Aromobatidae) from Sierra de Aroa, northwestern Venezuela, and an amended definition and call description of M. herminae (Boettger, 1893). Zootaxa. 2018;4461: 451–476. pmid:30314062
  16. 16. Kenny J. The Amphibia of Trinidad. Studies on the Fauna of Curacao and other Caribbean Islands. 1969;1: 1–78.
  17. 17. Wells KD. Social behavior and communication of a dendrobatid frog (Colostethus trinitatis). Herpetologica. 1980; 189–199.
  18. 18. Olson V, Owens I. Costly sexual signals: are carotenoids rare, risky or required? Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 1998;13: 510–514.
  19. 19. Richardson C, Lena J-P, Joly P, Lengagne T. Are leaders good mates? A study of call timing and male quality in a chorus situation. Anim Behav. 2008;76: 1487–1495.
  20. 20. Greener , Shepard R, Hoskisson P, Asmath H, Downie J. How many Trinidad stream frogs (Mannophryne trinitatis) are there, and should they be regarded as vulnerable to extinction? Herp J. 2017;27: 5–11.
  21. 21. STEVENS M, PÁRRAGA AC, CUTHILL IC, PRIDGE JC, TOTROSCIANKO . Using digital photography to study animal coloration. Biol J Linn Soc. 2007;90: 211–237.
  22. 22. BERGMAN TJ, BEEHNER JC. A simple method for measuring colour in wild animals: validation and use on chest patch colour in geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Biol J Linn Soc. 2008;94: 231–240.
  23. 23. Team CR. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2019.
  24. 24. Downie J, Smith J. Survival of Larval Leptodactylus fuscus (Anura: Leptodactylidae) out of Water: Developmental Differences and Interspecific Comparisons. J Herpetol. 2003;37: 107–115.
  25. 25. Smith J, Downie J, Bryce R. Metamorphic duration: an under‐studied variable in frog life histories. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2004;83: 261–272.
  26. 26. Thomson R, Hoskisson P, Brozio S, Downie J. Apparent lack of chytrid infection in northeast Tobago’s frogs. Living World, Journal of the Trinidad and Tobago Field Naturalists’ Club. 2018; 82–86.
  27. 27. Mijares-Urrutia A, Arends A. Un nuevo Mannophryne (Anura: Dendrobatidae) del estado Falcón, con comentarios sobre la conservación del género en el noroeste de Venezuela. Caribbean Journal of Science. 1999;35: 231–237.
  28. 28. La Marca E. Taxonomy of the Frogs of the Genus Mannophryne (Amphibia, Anura, Dendrobatidae). Asociación Amigos de Doñana. 1994;
  29. 29. Mijares-Urrutia A, Arends A. A new Mannophryne (Anura: Dendrobatidae) from western Venezuela, with comments on the generic allocation of Colostethus larandinus. Herpetologica. 1999; 106–114.
  30. 30. Yústiz E. Un nuevo Colostethus (Amphibia: Dendrobatidae) de la Sierra de Barbacoas, Estado Lara, Venezuela. Bioagro. 1991;3: 145–150.
  31. 31. Manzanilla J, La Marca E, Jowers M, Sánchez D, París M. Un nuevo mannophryne (amphibia: anura: Dendrobatidae) del macizo del Turimiquire, noreste De Venezuela. Herpetotropicos. 2005;2.
  32. 32. Hardy J. A new frog of the genus Colostethus from the island of Tobago (Anura: Dendrobatidae). Bulletin of the Maryland Herpetological Society. 1983;19: 47–57.
  33. 33. Dempewolf L, Hailey A, Mannette R, Cazabon M, Roach A, Jahson AI, et al. Ecological observations on the Critically Endangered Tobago endemic frog Mannophryne olmonae. Appl Herpetol. 2007;4: 377–386.
  34. 34. Barrio-Amorós CL, Rivas GA, Molina C, Santos J, Kaiser H. Intraspecific variation in the endangered frog Mannophryne riveroi (Anura, Dendrobatidae, Aromobatinae), with comments on coloration and natural history. Herpetological Notes. 2010;3: 151–160.
  35. 35. La Marca E, Vargas Galarce JY. A NEW SPECIES OF COLLARED FROG (AMPHIBIA: ANURA: AROMOBATIDAE: MANNOPHRYNE) FROM THE ANDES OF TRUJILLO STATE, VENEZUELA. Herpetotropicos. 2009;3: 1–7.
  36. 36. Vargas Galarce JY, La Marca E. A new species of collared frog (Amphibia: Anura: Aromobatidae: Mannophryne) from the Andes of Trujillo state, Venezuela. Herpetotropicos. 2006;3.
  37. 37. Manzanilla J, Jowers M, La Marca E, García-París M. Taxonomic reassessment of Mannophryne trinitatis (Anura: Dendrobatidae) with a description of a new species from Venezuela. The Herpetological Journal. 2007;17: 31–42.
  38. 38. La Marca E. A new species of collared frog (Anura: Dendrobatidae: Colostethus) from Serrania de Portuguesa, Andes of Estado Lara, Venezuela. Amphibia-reptilia. 1989;10: 175–183.
  39. 39. Kindermann C, Narayan EJ, Hero J-M. The Neuro-Hormonal Control of Rapid Dynamic Skin Colour Change in an Amphibian during Amplexus. Plos One. 2014;9: e114120. pmid:25470775
  40. 40. AmphibiaWeb. Information on amphibian biology and conservation. 2019;
  41. 41. MANZANILLA J, La MARCA E, GARCÍA-PARÍS M. Phylogenetic patterns of diversification in a clade of Neotropical frogs (Anura: Aromobatidae: Mannophryne). Biol J Linn Soc. 2009;97: 185–199.
  42. 42. Veith M, Kosuch J, Rödel M-O, Hillers A, Schmitz A, Burger M, et al. Multiple evolution of sexual dichromatism in African reed frogs. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2009;51: 388–393. pmid:19166951
  43. 43. Durant P, Dole JW. Aggressive Behavior in Colostethus (= Prostherapis) collaris (Anura: Dendrobatidae). Herpetologica. 1975;31: 23–26.
  44. 44. Stangel J, Preininger D, Sztatecsny M, Hödl W. Ontogenetic Change of Signal Brightness in the Foot-Flagging Frog Species Staurois parvus and Staurois guttatus. Herpetologica. 2015;71: 1–7. pmid:25983337
  45. 45. Gomez D, Richardson C, Lengagne T, Plenet S, Joly P, Léna J-P, et al. The role of nocturnal vision in mate choice: females prefer conspicuous males in the European tree frog (Hyla arborea). Proc Biological Sci Royal Soc. 2009;276: 2351–8. pmid:19324736
  46. 46. Royan A, Muir A, Downie J. Variability in escape trajectory in the Trinidadian stream frog and two treefrogs at different life-history stages. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2010;88: 922–934.
  47. 47. Downie J, Robinson E, Linklater‐McLennan R, Somerville E, Kamenos N. Are there costs to extended larval transport in the Trinidadian stream frog, Mannophryne trinitatis (Dendrobatidae)? J Nat Hist. 2005;39: 2023–2034.
  48. 48. Downie J, Livingstone S, Cormack J. Selection of tadpole deposition sites by male Trinidadian stream frogs, Mannophryne trinitatis (Dendrobatidae): an example of anti-predator behaviour. Herpetological Journal. 2001;11: 91–100.
  49. 49. Lehtinen RM, Georgiadis AP. Observations on parental care in the glass frog Hyalinobatrachium orientale (Anura: Centrolenidae) from Tobago, with comments on its natural history. Phyllomedusa J Herpetology. 2012;11: 75–77.